Opinion poll

History

The first known example of an opinion poll was a local straw poll conducted by The Harrisburg Pennsylvanian in 1824, showing Andrew Jackson leading John Quincy Adams by 335 votes to 169 in the contest for the United States Presidency. Such straw votes gradually became more popular, but they remained local, usually city-wide phenomena. In 1916, the Literary Digest embarked on a national survey (partly as a circulation-raising exercise) and correctly predicted Woodrow Wilson’s election as president. Mailing out millions of postcards and simply counting the returns, the Digest correctly called the following four presidential elections.

In 1936 however the Digest came unstuck. Its 2.3 million “voters” constituted a huge sample; however they were generally more affluent Americans who tended to have Republican sympathies. The Literary Digest was ignorant of this new bias. The week before election day, it reported that Alf Landon was far more popular than Franklin D. Roosevelt. At the same time, George Gallup conducted a far smaller, but more scientifically-based survey, in which he polled a demographically representative sample. Gallup correctly predicted Roosevelt’s landslide victory. The Literary Digest soon went out of business, while polling started to take off.

Elmo Roper was another American pioneer in political forecasting using scientific polls. He predicted the reelection of President Franklin D. Roosevelt three times, in 1936, 1940, and 1944. Louis Harris had been in the field of public opinion since 1947 when he joined the Elmo Roper firm then later became partner.

Gallup launched a subsidiary in the United Kingdom, where it correctly predicted Labour’s victory in the 1945 general election, in contrast with virtually all other commentators, who expected a victory for the Conservative Party, led by Winston Churchill.

By the 1950s, various types of polling had spread to most democracies. In Iraq, surveys conducted soon after the 2003 war have aimed to measure the true feelings of Iraqi citizens to Saddam Hussein, post-war conditions, and the presence of US forces.

Sample and polling methods

The text in this article or section may be incoherent or very hard to understand, and should be reworded if the intended meaning can be determined. The talk page may have details.

Opinion polls for many years were maintained through telecommunications or in person-to-person contact. Methods and techniques vary, though they are widely accepted in most areas. Verbal, ballot, and processed types can be conducted efficiently, contrasted with other types of surveys, systematics, and complicated matrices beyond previous orthodox procedures. Opinion polling developed into popular applications through popular thought, although response rates for some surveys declined. Also, the following has also led to differentiating results: Some polling organizations, such as and Angus Reid Strategies, YouGov, Rate Your Politician LLP and Zogby use Internet surveys, where a sample is drawn from a large panel of volunteers, and the results are weighed to reflect the demographics of the population of interest. This is in contrast to popular web polls that draw on whomever wishes to participate, rather than a scientific sample of the population, and are therefore not generally considered professional.

Tracking polls

A tracking poll is a poll repeated at intervals generally averaged over a trailing window. For example, a weekly tracking poll uses the data from the past week and discards older data.

A key benefit of tracking polls is that the trend of a tracking poll (the change over time) corrects for bias: regardless of whether a poll consistently over or underestimates opinion, the trend correctly reflects increases or decreases.

A caution is that estimating the trend is more difficult and error-prone than estimating the level intuitively, if one estimates the change, the difference between two numbers X and Y, then one has to contend with the error in both X and Y it is not enough to simply take the difference, as the change may be random noise. For details, see t-test. A rough guide is that if the change in measurement falls outside the margin of error, it is worth attention.

Potential for inaccuracy

Polls based on samples of populations are subject to sampling error which reflects the effects of chance and uncertainty in the sampling process. The uncertainty is often expressed as a margin of error. The margin of error is usually defined as the radius of a confidence interval for a particular statistic from a survey. One example is the percent of people who prefer product A versus product B. When a single, global margin of error is reported for a survey, it refers to the maximum margin of error for all reported percentages using the full sample from the survey. If the statistic is a percentage, this maximum margin of error can be calculated as the radius of the confidence interval for a reported percentage of 50%. Others suggest that a poll with a random sample of 1,000 people has margin of sampling error of 3% for the estimated percentage of the whole population. A 3% margin of error means that if the same procedure is used a large number of times, 95% of the time the true population average will be within the 95% confidence interval of the sample estimate plus or minus 3%. The margin of error can be reduced by using a larger sample, however if a pollster wishes to reduce the margin of error to 1% they would need a sample of around 10,000 people. In practice pollsters need to balance the cost of a large sample against the reduction in sampling error and a sample size of around 5001,000 is a typical compromise for political polls. (Note that to get complete responses it may be necessary to include thousands of additional participators.) Another way to reduce the margin of error is to rely on poll averages. This makes the assumption that the procedure is similar enough between many different polls and uses the sample size of each poll to create a polling average. An example of a polling average can be found here: 2008 Presidential Election polling average. Another source of error stems from faulty demographic models by pollsters who weigh their samples by particular variables such as party identification in an election. For example, if you assume that the breakdown of the US population by party identification has not changed since the previous presidential election, you may underestimate a victory or a defeat of a particular party candidate that saw a surge or decline in its party registration relative to the previous presidential election cycle.

Over time, a number of theories and mechanisms have been offered to explain erroneous polling results. Some of these reflect errors on the part of the pollsters; many of them are statistical in nature. Others blame the respondents for not giving candid answers (e.g., the Bradley effect, the Shy Tory Factor); these can be more controversial.

Nonresponse bias

Since some people do not answer calls from strangers, or refuse to answer the poll, poll samples may not be representative samples from a population. Because of this selection bias, the characteristics of those who agree to be interviewed may be markedly different from those who decline. That is, the actual sample is a biased version of the universe the pollster wants to analyze. In these cases, bias introduces new errors, one way or the other, that are in addition to errors caused by sample size. Error due to bias does not become smaller with larger sample sizes, because taking a larger sample size simply repeats the same mistake on a larger scale. If the people who refuse to answer, or are never reached, have the same characteristics as the people who do answer, then the final results should be unbiased. If the people who do not answer have different opinions then there is bias in the results. In terms of election polls, studies suggest that bias effects are small, but each polling firm has its own formulas on how to adjust weights to minimize selection bias.

Response bias

Survey results may be affected by response bias, where the answers given by respondents do not reflect their true beliefs. This may be deliberately engineered by unscrupulous pollsters in order to generate a certain result or please their clients, but more often is a result of the detailed wording or ordering of questions (see below). Respondents may deliberately try to manipulate the outcome of a poll by e.g. advocating a more extreme position than they actually hold in order to boost their side of the argument or give rapid and ill-considered answers in order to hasten the end of their questioning. Respondents may also feel under social pressure not to give an unpopular answer. For example, respondents might be unwilling to admit to unpopular attitudes like racism or sexism, and thus polls might not reflect the true incidence of these attitudes in the population. In American political parlance, this a phenomenon is often referred to as the Bradley Effect. If the results of surveys are widely publicized this effect may be magnified – a phenomenon commonly referred to as the spiral of silence.

Wording of questions

It is well established that the wording of the questions, the order in which they are asked and the number and form of alternative answers offered can influence results of polls. For instance, the public is more likely to indicate support for a person who is described by the operator as one of the “leading candidates”. This support itself overrides subtle bias for one candidate, as is lumping some candidates in an “other” category or vice versa. 21st century Polling arms variate in complexity due to these circumstances. Thus comparisons between polls often boil down to the wording of the question. On some issues, question wording can result in quite pronounced differences between surveys. This can also, however, be a result of legitimately conflicted feelings or evolving attitudes, rather than a poorly constructed survey.

A common technique to control for this bias is to rotate the order in which questions are asked. Many pollsters also split-sample. This involves having two different versions of a question, with each version presented to half the respondents.

The most effective controls, used by attitude researchers, are:

asking enough questions to allow all aspects of an issue to be covered and to control effects due to the form of the question (such as positive or negative wording), the adequacy of the number being established quantitatively with psychometric measures such as reliability coefficients, and

analyzing the results with psychometric techniques which synthesize the answers into a few reliable scores and detect ineffective questions.

These controls are not widely used in the polling industry.

Coverage bias

Another source of error is the use of samples that are not representative of the population as a consequence of the methodology used, as was the experience of the Literary Digest in 1936. For example, telephone sampling has a built-in error because in many times and places, those with telephones have generally been richer than those without.

In some places many people have only mobile telephones. Because pollsters cannot call mobile phones (it is unlawful in the United States to make unsolicited calls to phones where the phone’s owner may be charged simply for taking a call), these individuals will never be included in the polling sample. If the subset of the population without cell phones differs markedly from the rest of the population, these differences can skew the results of the poll. Polling organizations have developed many weighting techniques to help overcome these deficiencies, to varying degrees of success. Studies of mobile phone users by the Pew Research Center in the US concluded that “cell-only respondents are different from landline respondents in important ways, (but) they were neither numerous enough nor different enough on the questions we examined to produce a significant change in overall general population survey estimates when included with the landline samples and weighted according to US Census parameters on basic demographic characteristics.”

This issue was first identified in 2004, but came to prominence only during the 2008 US presidential election. In previous elections, the proportion of the general population using cell phones was small, but as this proportion has increased, the worry is that polling only landlines is no longer representative of the general population. In 2003, a 2.9% of households were wireless (cellphones only) compared to 12.8 in 2006. This results in “coverage error”. Many polling organisations select their sample by dialling random telephone numbers; however, there is a clear tendency for polls which included mobile phones in their sample to show a much larger lead for Obama than polls that did not.

The potential sources of bias are:

Some households use cellphones only and have no landline. This tends to include minorities and younger voters; and occurs more frequently in metropolitan areas. Men are more likely to be cellphone-only compared to women.

Some people may not be contactable by landline from Monday to Friday and may be contactable only by cellphone.

Some people use their landlines only to access the Internet, and answer calls only to their cellphones.

Some polling companies have attempted to get around that problem by including a “cellphone supplement”. There are a number of problems with including cellphones in a telephone poll:

It is difficult to get co-operation from cellphone users, because in many parts of the US, users are charged for both outgoing and incoming calls. That means that pollsters have had to offer financial compensation to gain co-operation.

US federal law prohibits the use of automated dialling devices to call cellphones (Telephone Consumer Protection Act of 1991). Numbers therefore have to be dialled by hand, which is more time-consuming and expensive for pollsters.

An oft-quoted example of opinion polls succumbing to errors was the UK General Election of 1992. Despite the polling organizations using different methodologies virtually all the polls in the lead up to the vote, and to a lesser extent exit polls taken on voting day, showed a lead for the opposition Labour party but the actual vote gave a clear victory to the ruling Conservative party.

In their deliberations after this embarrassment the pollsters advanced several ideas to account for their errors, including:

Late swing 

Voters who changed their minds shortly before voting tended to favour the Conservatives, so the error was not as great as it first appeared.

Nonresponse bias 

Conservative voters were less likely to participate in surveys than in the past and were thus under-represented.

The Shy Tory Factor 

The Conservatives had suffered a sustained period of unpopularity as a result of economic difficulties and a series of minor scandals, leading to a spiral of silence in which some Conservative supporters were reluctant to disclose their sincere intentions to pollsters.

The relative importance of these factors was, and remains, a matter of controversy, but since then the polling organizations have adjusted their methodologies and have achieved more accurate results in subsequent elections.

Polling organizations

In Australia the most notable companies are:

Newspoll – published in News Limited’s The Australian newspaper

Roy Morgan Research – published in the Crikey email reporting service

Galaxy Polling – published in News Limited’s tabloid papers

AC Nielsen Polling – published in Fairfax newspapers

Ipsos and I-View

In Canada the most notable companies are:

Angus Reid Strategies

EKOS Research Associates

Environics Research Group

Harris/Decima

Ipsos-Reid

Lger Marketing

Nanos Research

In Egypt, the most notable polling organization is

Opinion Poll Center

In Jordan the dominant organization is:

Knowledge World Center for Polls

In Iran, some notable polling organisations include:

Ayandeh – closed in 2002 and director Abbas Abdi arrested

In Nigeria the most notable polling organization is:

NOI-Gallup poll

In South Africa the most notable company is:

Ipsos Markinor who have conducted opinion polls since 1976.

In the Ukraine, the most notable pollsters are:

Research & Branding Group, widely published throughout Ukraine and Internationally. Works include exit polls and regular surveys of the public’s political opinions

Razumkov Centre A policy think tank also widely published throughout Ukraine

SOCIS (Socis center for social and political studies)

In the United Kingdom, the most notable pollsters are:

ComRes, retained pollster for the BBC and The Independent

Ipsos MORI (formerly MORI).

YouGov, an online pollster.

R8 YourPolitician Ltd, an online as well as traditional pollster organisation.

GfK NOP

ICM

Populus, official The Times pollster

In the United States, some notable companies include:

Gallup poll run by The Gallup Organization

Harris Poll

Ipsos

National Opinion Research Center

Nielsen ratings

Pew Research Center

Rasmussen Reports

Zogby International

In Spain:

AKRA DELTA

Ipsos

All the major television networks, alone or in conjunction with the largest newspapers or magazines, in virtually every country with elections, operate their own versions of polling operations, in collaboration or independently through various applications. One of the applications can be found on Facebook

Several organizations try to monitor the behavior of polling firms and the use of polling and statistical data, including the Pew Research Center and, in Canada, the Laurier Institute for the Study of Public Opinion and Policy.

Failures

The best-known failure of opinion polling to date in the United States was the prediction that Thomas Dewey would defeat Harry S. Truman in the 1948 US presidential election. Major polling organizations, including Gallup and Roper, indicated a landslide victory for Dewey.

In the United Kingdom, most polls failed to predict the Conservative election victories of 1970 and 1992, and Labour’s victory in 1974. However, their figures at other elections have been generally accurate.

Influence

By providing information about voting intentions, opinion polls can sometimes influence the behavior of electors, and in his book The Broken Compass, Peter Hitchens asserts that opinion polls are actually a device for influencing public opinion. The various theories about how this happens can be split up into two groups: bandwagon/underdog effects, and strategic (“tactical”) voting.

A bandwagon effect occurs when the poll prompts voters to back the candidate shown to be winning in the poll. The idea that voters are susceptible to such effects is old, stemming at least from 1884; reported that it was first used in a political cartoon in the magazine Puck in that year. It has also remained persistent in spite of a lack of empirical corroboration until the late 20th century. George Gallup spent much effort in vain trying to discredit this theory in his time by presenting empirical research. A recent meta-study of scientific research on this topic indicates that from the 1980s onward the Bandwagon effect is found more often by researchers.

The opposite of the bandwagon effect is the underdog effect. It is often mentioned in the media. This occurs when people vote, out of sympathy, for the party perceived to be “losing” the elections. There is less empirical evidence for the existence of this effect than there is for the existence of the bandwagon effect.

The second category of theories on how polls directly affect voting is called strategic or tactical voting. This theory is based on the idea that voters view the act of voting as a means of selecting a government. Thus they will sometimes not choose the candidate they prefer on ground of ideology or sympathy, but another, less-preferred, candidate from strategic considerations. An example can be found in the United Kingdom general election, 1997. As he was then a Cabinet Minister, Michael Portillo’s constituency of Enfield Southgate was believed to be a safe seat but opinion polls showed the Labour candidate Stephen Twigg steadily gaining support, which may have prompted undecided voters or supporters of other parties to support Twigg in order to remove Portillo. Another example is the boomerang effect where the likely supporters of the candidate shown to be winning feel that chances are slim and that their vote is not required, thus allowing another candidate to win.

These effects indicate how opinion polls can directly affect political choices of the electorate. But directly or indirectly, other effects can be surveyed and analyzed on on all political parties. The form of media framing and party ideology shifts must also be taken under consideration. Opinion polling in some instances is a measure of cognitive bias, which is variably considered and handled appropriately in its various applications.

See also

British Polling Council

Claude E. Robinson

Confidence interval

Deliberative opinion poll

Enterprise Feedback Management

European Survey Research Association

Exit poll

Franklin & Marshall College Poll

George Gallup

Missouri bellwether

Nationwide opinion polling for the United States presidential election, 2008

Ochlocracy

Open access poll

Psepholograph

Push poll

Research & Branding Group, Ukrainian Public Opinion Polls

Shy Tory Factor

Straw poll

United States Presidential approval rating

Urtak

Russian Public Opinion Research Center

Footnotes

^ a b Cantril, Hadley and Mildred Strunk (1951). Public Opinion, 1935-1946. Princeton University Press. p. vii.

^ About the Tracking Polls

^ publicagenda.org

^ Lynch, Scott M. Introduction to Bayesian Statistics and Estimation for Social Scientists (2007)

^ abcnews.go.com

^ http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Opinion_polling#cite_note-1

^ Public Agenda Issue Guide: Higher Education – Public View – Red Flags | Public Agenda

^ Public Agenda Issue Guide: Gay Rights – Public View – Red Flags | Public Agenda

^ Public Agenda Issue Guide: Abortion – Public View – Red Flags | Public Agenda

^ The Seven Stages of Public Opinion | Public Agenda

^ Keeter, Scott (2007-06-27). “How Serious Is Polling’s Cell-Only Problem?”. Pew Research Center Publications. http://pewresearch.org/pubs/515/polling-cell-only-problem. 

^ Blumenthal, Mark (2008-09-19). “More Pollsters Interviewing By Cell Phone”. Pollster.com. http://www.pollster.com/blogs/more_pollsters_interviewing_by.php. Retrieved 2008-11-04. 

^ Blumenthal, Mark (2008-07-17). “New Pew data on cell phones”. Pollster. http://www.pollster.com/blogs/new_pew_data_on_cell_phones.php. Retrieved 2008-11-04. 

^ Blumberg SJ, Luke JV (2007-05-14) (PDF). Wireless Substitution: Early Release of Estimates Based on Data from the National Health Interview Survey, Julyecember 2006. Centers for Disease Control. http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhis/earlyrelease/wireless200705.pdf. Retrieved 2009-06-22. 

^ Silver, Nate (2008-11-02). “The Cellphone effect, continued”. FiveThirtyEight.com. http://www.fivethirtyeight.com/2008/11/cellphone-effect-continued.html. Retrieved 2008-11-04. 

^ Blumenthal, Mark (2008-10-17). “More Cell Phone Data from Gallup”. Pollster.com. http://www.pollster.com/blogs/more_cell_phone_data_from_gall.php. Retrieved 2008-11-04. 

^ Silver, Nate (2008-07-22). “The Cellphone Problem, Revisited”. FiveThirtyEight.com. http://www.fivethirtyeight.com/2008/07/cellphone-problem-revisited.html. Retrieved 2008-11-04. 

^ The Public Opinion Poll Center

^ Alert. Journalist Abdollah Nouri released but another journalist arrested

^ “Research&Branding Group Poll: 26% Of Ukrainians Prepared To Support Yanukovych For President”. The FINANCIAL website. August 19, 2009. http://www.finchannel.com/Main_News/Ukraine/45385_Research&Branding_Group_Poll:_26%25_Of_Ukrainians_Prepared_To_Support_Yanukovych_For_President/. Retrieved 2010-02-11. 

^ Poll: “CHANGE OF ELECTORAL SITUATION IN UKRAINE – June 2009”, Research & Branding Group (June, 2009)

^ (Ukrainian) 2008 . (), Razumkov Centre (December 26, 2008)

^ Socis Poll: 25% Of Ukrainians Prepared To Support Yanukovych For President, 20.5% To Vote For Tymoshenko, Ukrainian News (August 17, 2009)

^ Facebook | Poll

^ Laurier Institute for the Study of Public Opinion and Policy

^ Hitchens, Peter (2009). “Chapter 1, Guy Fawkes Gets a Blackberry”. The Broken Compass: How British Politics Lost its Way. Continuum International Publishing Group Ltd. ISBN 1847064051. 

^ Safire (1993: 43)

^ a b Irwin, Galen A. and Joop J. M. Van Holsteyn. Bandwagons, Underdogs, the Titanic and the Red Cross: The Influence of Public Opinion Polls on Voters (2000).

External references

Asher, Herbert: Polling and the Public. What Every Citizen Should Know, fourth edition. Washington, D.C.: CQ Press, 1998.

Bourdieu, Pierre, “Public Opinion does not exist” in Sociology in Question, London, Sage (1995)

Bradburn, Norman M. and Seymour Sudman. Polls and Surveys: Understanding What They Tell Us (1988)

Cantril, Hadley. Gauging Public Opinion (1944)

Cantril, Hadley and Mildred Strunk, eds. Public Opinion, 1935-1946 (1951), massive compilation of many public opinion polls from US, UK, Canada, Australia, and elsewhere.

Converse, Jean M. Survey Research in the United States: Roots and Emergence 1890-1960 (1987), the standard history

Crespi, Irving. Public Opinion, Polls, and Democracy (1989)

Gallup, George. Public Opinion in a Democracy (1939)

Gallup, Alec M. ed. The Gallup Poll Cumulative Index: Public Opinion, 1935-1997 (1999) lists 10,000+ questions, but no results

Gallup, George Horace, ed. The Gallup Poll; Public Opinion, 1935-1971 3 vol (1972) summarizes results of each poll.

Glynn, Carroll J., Susan Herbst, Garrett J. O’Keefe, and Robert Y. Shapiro. Public Opinion (1999) textbook

Lavrakas, Paul J. et al. eds. Presidential Polls and the News Media (1995)

Moore, David W. The Superpollsters: How They Measure and Manipulate Public Opinion in America (1995)

Niemi, Richard G., John Mueller, Tom W. Smith, eds. Trends in Public Opinion: A Compendium of Survey Data (1989)

Oskamp, Stuart and P. Wesley Schultz; Attitudes and Opinions (2004)

Robinson, Claude E. Straw Votes (1932).

Robinson, Matthew Mobocracy: How the Media’s Obsession with Polling Twists the News, Alters Elections, and Undermines Democracy (2002)

Rogers, Lindsay. The Pollsters: Public Opinion, Politics, and Democratic Leadership (1949)

Traugott, Michael W. The Voter’s Guide to Election Polls 3rd ed. (2004)

James G. Webster, Patricia F. Phalen, Lawrence W. Lichty; Ratings Analysis: The Theory and Practice of Audience Research Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2000

Young, Michael L. Dictionary of Polling: The Language of Contemporary Opinion Research (1992)

Additional Sources

Walden, Graham R. Survey Research Methodology, 1990-1999: An Annotated Bibliography. Bibliographies and Indexes in Law and Political Science Series. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, Greenwood Publishing Group, Inc., 2002. xx, 432p.

Walden, Graham R. Public Opinion Polls and Survey Research: A Selective Annotated Bibliography of U.S. Guides and Studies from the 1980s. Public Affairs and Administrative Series, edited by James S. Bowman, vol. 24. New York, NY: Garland Publishing Inc., 1990. xxix, 360p.

Walden, Graham R. Polling and Survey Research Methods 1935-1979: An Annotated Bibliography. Bibliographies and Indexes in Law and Political Science Series, vol. 25. Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing Group, Inc., 1996. xxx, 581p.

External links

Polls from UCB Libraries GovPubs

The Pew Research Center nonpartisan “fact tank” providing information on the issues, attitudes and trends shaping America and the world by conducting public opinion polling and social science research

“Use Opinion Research To Build Strong Communication” by Frank Noto

Public Agenda nonpartisan, nonprofit group that tracks public opinion data in the United States

National Council on Public Polls association of polling organizations in the United States devoted to setting high professional standards for surveys

USA Election Polls tracks the public opinion polls related to elections in the US

Survey Analysis Tool based on A. Berkopec, HyperQuick algorithm for discrete hypergeometric distribution, Journal of Discrete Algorithms, Elsevier, 2006.

v  d  e

Social surveys

Types of surveys

Census  Hypothesis testing  Market research  Opinion poll

Acquiring information

Sampling  Questionnaire  Interview (Structured, Semi-structured and Unstructured)

Interpreting information

Level of measurement  Statistics  Variable

Notable examples

Gallup poll  General Social Survey  International Social Survey  United Kingdom Census  United States Census  National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey

Category  Project

Categories: Types of polling | Psychometrics | Public opinion | Sampling (statistics)

I am an expert from International Trade Community, usually analyzes all kind of industries situation, such as extruded foam board , air king exhaust fans.

Processing your request, Please wait....

Leave a Reply